Remember
the Mozart effect? Thanks to a suggestion in 1993 that listening to Mozart
makes you cleverer, there has been a flood of compilation CDs filled with
classical tunes that will allegedly boost your baby's brain power.
Yet
there's no evidence for this claim, and indeed the original 'Mozart effect'
paper did not make it. It reported a slight, short-term performance enhancement
in some spatial tasks when preceded by listening to Mozart as opposed to
sitting in silence. Some follow-up studies replicated the effect, others did
not. None found it specific to Mozart; one study showed that pop music could
have the same effect on schoolchildren. It seems this curious but marginal
effect stems from the cognitive benefits of any enjoyable auditory stimulus, which
need not even be musical.
The
original claim doubtless had such inordinate impact because it plays to a
long-standing suspicion that music makes you smarter. And as neuroscientists
Nina Kraus and Bharath Chandrasekaran of Northwestern University in Evanston,
Illinois, point out in a review published today in Nature Reviews
Neuroscience3, there is good evidence that music training reshapes the brain in
ways that convey broader cognitive benefits. It can, they say, lead to
"changes throughout the auditory system that prime musicians for listening
challenges beyond music processing".
This
is no surprise. Many sorts of mental training and learning alter the brain,
just as physical training alters the body, and learning-related structural
differences between the brains of musicians and non-musicians are well
established. Moreover, both neurological and psychological tests show that
music processing draws on cognitive resources that are not music-specific, such
as pitch processing, memory and pattern recognition— so cultivating these
mental functions through music would naturally be expected to have a wider
pay-off. The interactions are two-way: the pitch sensitivity imbued by tonal
languages such as Mandarin Chinese, for example, enhances the ability to name a
musical note just from hearing it (called absolute pitch).
We
can hardly be surprised, meanwhile, that music lessons improve children's IQ,
given that they will nourish general faculties such as memory, coordination and
attentiveness. Kraus and Chandrasekaran now point out that, thanks to the
brain's plasticity (the ability to 'rewire' itself), musical training sharpens
our sensitivity to pitch, timing and timbre, and as a result our capacity to
discern emotional intonation in speech, to learn our native and foreign
languages, and to identify statistical regularities in abstract sound stimuli.
Music
to our ears
Yet
all these benefits of music education have done rather little to alter a common
perception that music is an optional extra to be offered only if children have
the time and inclination. Ethnomusicologist John Blacking put it more
damningly: we insist that musicality is a rare gift, so that music is to be
created by a tiny minority for the passive consumption of the majority. Having
spent years among African cultures that recognized no such distinctions,
Blacking was appalled at the way this elitism labelled most people 'unmusical'.
Kraus
and Chandrasekaran rightly argue that the marginalization of music training in
schools "should be reassessed" in the light of the benefits it may
offer by "improving learning skills and listening ability". But it
will be a sad day when the only way to persuade educationalists to embrace
music is via its side effects on cognition and intelligence. We should be especially
wary of that argument in this age of cost-benefit analyses, targets and
utilitarian impact assessments. Music should indeed be celebrated (and studied)
as a gymnasium for the mind; but ultimately its value lies with the way it
enriches, socializes and humanizes us qua music
And
while in no way detracting from the validity of the call for music to be
essential in education, it's significant that musical training, like any other
pleasure, has its hazards when taken to excess. I was recently privileged to
discuss with the pianist Leon Fleisher his traumatic but fascinating struggle
with focal dystonia, a condition that results in localized loss of muscle
control. Fleisher's dazzling career as a concert pianist was almost ended in
the early 1960s when he found that two fingers of his right hand insisted on
curling up. After several decades of teaching and one-handed playing, Fleisher
regained the use of both hands through a regime of deep massage and injections
of botox to relax the muscles. But he says his condition is still present, and
he must constantly battle against it.
Focal
dystonia is not a muscular problem (like cramp) but a neural one: over-training
disrupts the feedback between muscles and brain, expanding the representation
of the hand in the sensory cortex until the neural correlates of the fingers
blur. It is the dark side of neural plasticity, and not so uncommon — an
estimated one in a hundred professional musicians suffer from it, although some
do so in secrecy, fearful of admitting to the debilitating problem.
We
would be hugely impoverished without virtuosi such as Fleisher. But his plight
serves as a reminder that hot-housing has its dangers, not only for the
performers but (as Blacking) suggests for the rest of us. Give us fine music,
but rough music too.
No comments:
Post a Comment
give ur feedback here!